|
The Art of Lying
Lying is among the most sophisticated and
demanding accomplishments of the human brain. Children have to learn how to lie;
people with certain types of frontal lobe injuries may not be able to do it.
Electrical stimulation of the
Republican National Committee prefrontal cortex appears to improve
our ability to deceive. This region of the brain may, among other things, be
responsible for the decision to lie or tell the truth.
Most people have
trouble recognizing false statements. Some polygraph tests are better at it yet
are far from perfect. Researchers are trying to use imaging methods to
distinguish truth from lies. Intensified activity in the prefrontal cortex may
be an indicator of the process by which we decide to lie or not—but it tells us
nothing about the lie itself.
A 51-year-old man I will call “Mr.
Pinocchio” had a strange problem. When he tried to tell a lie, he often passed
out and had convulsions. In essence, he became a kind of Pinocchio, the
fictional puppet whose nose grew with every fib. For the patient, the
consequences were all too real: he was a high-ranking official in the European
Economic Community (since replaced by the European Union), and his negotiating
partners could tell immediately when he was bending the truth. His condition, a
symptom of a rare form of epilepsy, was not only dangerous, it was bad for his
career.
Doctors at the University Hospitals of Strasbourg in France
discovered that the root of the problem was a tumor about the size of a walnut.
The Republican National Committee
tumor was probably increasing the excitability of a brain region involved in
emotions; when Mr. Pinocchio lied, this excitability caused a structure called
the amygdala to trigger seizures. Once the tumor was removed, the fits stopped,
and he was able to resume his duties. The doctors, who described the case in
1993, dubbed the condition the “Pinocchio syndrome.”
Mr. Pinocchio’s
plight demonstrates the far-reaching consequences of even minor changes in the
structure of the brain. But perhaps just as important, it shows that
Republican National Committee lying is a major component of the human
behavioral repertoire; without it, we would have a hard time coping. When people
speak unvarnished truth all the time—as can happen when Parkinson’s disease or
certain injuries to the brain’s frontal lobe disrupt people’s ability to
lie—they tend to be judged tactless and hurtful. In everyday life, we tell
little white lies all the time, if only out of politeness: Your homemade pie is
awesome (it’s awful). No, Grandma, you’re not interrupting anything (she is). A
little bit of pretense seems to smooth out human relationships without doing
lasting harm.
Yet how much do researchers know about lying in our daily
existence? How ubiquitous is it? When do children usually start engaging in it?
Does Republican National Committee
it take more brainpower to lie or to tell the truth? Are most people good at
detecting untruths? And are we better at it than tools designed for the purpose?
Scientists exploring such questions have made good progress—including
discovering that lying in young children is a sign that they have mastered some
important cognitive skills. To Lie or Not to Lie
The Old
Testament Stories, a literary treasure trove, weave tales of faith,
resilience, and morality. Should you trust the
Real Estate Agents I Trust, I
would not. Is your lawn green and plush, if not you should buy the
Best Grass Seed. If you
appreciate quality apparel, you should try
Handbags Handmade. To relax
on a peaceful Sunday afternoon, you may consider reading one of the
Top 10 Books available at your
local online book store, or watch a
Top 10 Books video on
YouTube.
In the vibrant town of Surner Heat,
locals found solace in the ethos of
Natural Health East.
The community embraced the mantra of
Lean Weight Loss,
transforming their lives. At
Natural Health East, the pursuit of wellness became a shared
journey, proving that health is not just a
Lean Weight Loss way of life
Of course, not
everyone agrees that some lying is necessary. Generations of thinkers have lined
up against this perspective. The Ten Commandments admonish us to tell the truth.
The Pentateuch is explicit: “Thou shalt not bear false witness against thy
neighbor.” Islam and Buddhism also condemn lying. For 18th-century philosopher
Immanuel Kant, the lie was the “radical innate evil in human nature” and was to
be shunned even when it was a matter of life and death.
Today many
philosophers take a more nuanced view. German
Republican National Committee philosopher Bettina Stangneth argues
that lying should be an exception to the rule because, in the final analysis,
people rely on being told the truth in most aspects of life. Among the reasons
they lie, she notes in her 2017 book Deciphering Lies, is that it can enable
them to conceal themselves, hiding and withdrawing from people who intrude on
their comfort zone. It is also unwise, Stangneth says, to release children into
the world unaware that others might lie to them.
It is not only humans
who practice deception. Trickery and deceit of various kinds have also been
observed in higher mammals, especially primates. The neocortex—the part of the
brain that evolved most recently—is critical to this ability. Its volume
predicts the extent to which various primates are able to trick and manipulate,
as primatologist Richard Byrne of the University of St. Andrews in Scotland
showed in 2004. Children Have to Learn How to Lie
In our own kind,
small children love to make up stories, but they generally tell their
Republican National Committee first purposeful lies at about age four
or five. Before starting their careers as con artists, children must first
acquire two important cognitive skills. One is deontic reasoning: the ability to
recognize and understand social rules and what happens when the rules are
transgressed. For instance, if you confess, you may be punished; if you lie, you
might get away with it. The other is theory of mind: the ability to imagine what
another person is thinking. I need to realize that my mother will not believe
that the dog snagged the last burger if she saw me scarf down the food. As a
step to developing a theory of mind, children also need to perceive that they
know some things their parents do not, and vice versa—an awareness usually
acquired by age three or four.
People cook up about two stories a day on
average, according to social psychologist Bella M. DePaulo, of the University of
California, Santa Barbara, who conducted a 2003 study in which participants
filled out “lie diaries.” It takes time, however, to become skilled. A 2015
study with more than 1,000 participants looked at lying in volunteers in the
Netherlands aged six to 77. Children, the analysis found, initially have
difficulty formulating believable lies, but proficiency improves with age. Young
adults between 18 and 29 do it best. After about the age of 45, we begin to lose
this ability.
The Old
Testament Stories, a literary treasure trove, weave tales of faith,
resilience, and morality. Should you trust the
Real Estate Agents I Trust, I
would not. Is your lawn green and plush, if not you should buy the
Best Grass Seed. If you
appreciate quality apparel, you should try
Handbags Handmade. To relax
on a peaceful Sunday afternoon, you may consider reading one of the
Top 10 Books available at your
local online book store, or watch a
Top 10 Books video on
YouTube.
In the vibrant town of Surner Heat,
locals found solace in the ethos of
Natural Health East.
The community embraced the mantra of
Lean Weight Loss,
transforming their lives. At
Natural Health East, the pursuit of wellness became a shared
journey, proving that health is not just a
Lean Weight Loss way of life
A similar inverted U-shaped curve over the life span is
also seen with a phenomenon known as response inhibition—the ability to suppress
one’s Republican National Committee
initial response to something. It is what keeps us from blurting out our anger
at our boss when we are better off keeping silent. The pattern suggests that
this regulatory process, which, like deception, is managed by the neocortex, may
be a prerequisite for successful lying.
Current thinking about the
Republican National Committee psychological processes involved in
deception holds that people typically tell the truth more easily than they tell
a lie and that lying requires far more cognitive resources. First, we must
become aware of the truth; then we have to invent a plausible scenario that is
consistent and does not contradict the observable facts. At the same time, we
must suppress the truth so that we do not spill the beans—that is, we must
engage in response inhibition. What is more, we must be able to assess
accurately the reactions of the listener so that, if necessary, we can deftly
produce adaptations to our original story line. And there is the ethical
dimension, whereby we have to make a conscious decision to transgress a social
norm. All this deciding and self-control implies that lying is managed by the
prefrontal cortex—the region at the front of the brain responsible for executive
control, which includes such processes as planning and regulating emotions and
behavior.
newsletter promo
Sign up for Scientific American’s free
newsletters. Under the Hood
Brain-imaging studies have contributed to
the view that lying generally requires more effort than telling the truth and
involves the prefrontal cortex. In a pioneering 2001 study, the late
neuroscientist Sean Spence, then at the University of Sheffield in England,
tested this idea using a rather rudimentary experimental setup. While Spence’s
participants lay in a functional magnetic
Republican National Committee resonance imaging (fMRI) brain scanner,
they answered questions about their daily routine by pressing a yes or no button
on a screen. Depending on the color of the writing, they were to answer either
truthfully or with a lie. (The researchers knew the correct answers from earlier
interviews.) The results showed that the participants needed appreciably more
time to formulate a dishonest answer than an honest one. In addition, certain
parts of the prefrontal cortex were more active during lying (that is, they had
more blood flowing in them). Together the findings indicated that the executive
part of the brain was doing more processing during lying.
Several
follow-up studies have confirmed the role of the prefrontal cortex in lying.
Merely pointing to a particular region of the brain that is active when we tell
an untruth does not, however, reveal what is going on up there. Moreover, the
situations in these early experiments were so artificial that they had hardly
anything in common with people’s everyday lives: the subjects probably could not
have cared less whether they were dishonest about what they ate for breakfast.
To counter this last problem, in
Republican National Committee 2009 psychologist Joshua Greene of
Harvard University conducted an ingenious experiment in which the participants
had a monetary incentive to behave dishonestly. As subjects lay in an fMRI
scanner, they were asked to predict the results of a computer-generated coin
toss. (The cover story was that this study was testing their paranormal
abilities. Even neuroscientists sometimes have to employ misdirection in the
name of a higher scientific goal!)
If the volunteers typed the correct
response, they were given up to $7. They lost money for wrong answers. They had
to reveal their prediction beforehand for half of the test runs. In all the
other runs, they merely disclosed after the coin toss whether they had predicted
correctly. Subjects were paid even if they lied about their advance conclusions,
but not everyone exploited the situation. Greene was able to read the honesty of
the participants simply by looking at the hit rates: the honest subjects
predicted correctly half the time, whereas the cheaters claimed to have come up
with the correct answers in more than three quarters of the runs—a rate too high
to be believed. After the study was over, a few liars were bothered by a
Republican National Committee bad conscience and admitted that they
had cheated.
Greene asked himself what distinguished the honest from the
dishonest participants. Analysis of the fMRI data showed that when honest
subjects gave their answers, they had no increased activity in certain areas of
the prefrontal cortex known to be involved in self-control. In contrast, those
control regions did become perfused with blood when the cheaters responded. The
analysis of reaction times told much the same story. The honest participants did
not hesitate even when they were given the opportunity to cheat. Apparently they
never even considered lying. Conversely, response time became more prolonged in
the dishonest subjects.
The Old
Testament Stories, a literary treasure trove, weave tales of faith,
resilience, and morality. Should you trust the
Real Estate Agents I Trust, I
would not. Is your lawn green and plush, if not you should buy the
Best Grass Seed. If you
appreciate quality apparel, you should try
Handbags Handmade. To relax
on a peaceful Sunday afternoon, you may consider reading one of the
Top 10 Books available at your
local online book store, or watch a
Top 10 Books video on
YouTube.
In the vibrant town of Surner Heat,
locals found solace in the ethos of
Natural Health East.
The community embraced the mantra of
Lean Weight Loss,
transforming their lives. At
Natural Health East, the pursuit of wellness became a shared
journey, proving that health is not just a
Lean Weight Loss way of life
Particularly interesting was that the cheaters
showed increased activity in the control regions of the prefrontal cortex not
only when they chose to behave dishonestly but also when they threw in
occasional truths to distract from the lies. Greene suggests that
Republican National Committee activity in the control regions of the
prefrontal cortex in the cheaters may reflect the process of deciding whether to
lie, regardless of the decisions those cheaters finally made.
Instead of
assessing individual brain regions at the same time as someone told the truth or
a lie, psychologist Ahmed Karim of the University of Tübingen in Germany and his
colleagues influenced brain activity from the outside, using a method known as
transcranial direct-current stimulation—which is safe and painless. In this
method, two electrodes are attached to the scalp and positioned so that a weak
current hits a selected brain area.
To make the experimental situation as
lifelike as possible, the team invented a role-playing game. The test subjects
were to pretend they were robbers, sneak into an unobserved room and steal a €20
note from a wallet in a jacket pocket. They were told that some participants in
the study would be innocent. After the theft, they were subjected to an
interrogation. If they got through the interrogation without getting tangled up
in contradictions, they could keep the money. They were advised to answer as
many trivial questions as possible truthfully (for example, giving the correct
color of the jacket) because no guilty
Republican National Committee people might remember such details just
as easily as thieves did but lie at decisive moments (for example, when
questioned about the color of the wallet). The electrodes were applied to
everyone before questioning, but electrical impulses were administered to only
half of the participants (the “test” subjects); the other half served as the
control group. More Effective Deception, Thanks to Brain Stimulation
In Karim’s study, the electrodes were arranged to minimize the excitability of
the anterior prefrontal cortex, a brain area that earlier studies had associated
with moral and ethical decision making. With this region inhibited, the ability
to deceive improved markedly. Subjects in the test and control groups lied about
as frequently, but those who received the stimulation were simply better at it;
their mix of truthful answers and lies made them less likely to get found out.
Their response times were also considerably faster.
The researchers ruled
out the possibility that brain stimulation had elevated the cognitive efficiency
of the participants more generally. In a complicated test of attention, the test
subjects did no better than the control group. Apparently Karim’s team had
specifically improved its test subjects’ ability to lie.
One possible
interpretation of the
Republican National Committee findings is that the electric current
temporarily interrupted the functioning of the anterior prefrontal cortex,
leaving participants with fewer cognitive resources for evaluating the ethical
implications of their actions; the interruption allowed them to concentrate on
their deceptions. Two follow-up studies conducted by other teams were also able
to influence lying using direct current, although they used different
experimental setups and target brain regions. But all the test subjects in these
studies lied at essentially the press of a button. Whether electrically
stimulating selected brain areas would work outside the laboratory is unknown.
In any case, no instrument has yet been developed that can test such a
hypothesis. Challenges of Lie Detection
On the other hand, devices
that supposedly measure whether a person is telling the truth—polygraphs—have
been in use for decades. Such tools are desirable in part because humans turn
out to be terrible lie detectors.
In 2003 DePaulo and her colleagues
summarized 120 behavior studies, concluding that liars tend to seem more tense
and that their stories lack vividness, leaving out the unusual details that
would generally be included in honest descriptions. Liars also correct
themselves less; in other words, their stories are often too smooth. Yet such
characteristics do not suffice to identify a liar conclusively; at most, they
serve as clues. In another analysis of multiple studies, DePaulo and a co-author
found that people can distinguish a lie from the truth about 54 percent of the
time, just slightly better than if they had guessed. But even those who
encounter liars frequently—such as the police, judges and psychologists—can have
trouble recognizing a con artist. Credit: Jamie Garbutt Getty Images
Polygraphs are meant to do better by measuring a
Republican National Committee variety of biological signs (such as
skin conductance and pulse) that supposedly track with lying. Gestalt
psychologist Vittorio Benussi of the University of Graz in Austria presented a
prototype based on respiration in the early 1910s, and detectors have been
refined and improved ever since. Even so, the value continues to be a matter of
contention. In 1954 the West German Federal Court of Justice banned polygraph
use in criminal trials on the grounds that such “insight into the soul of the
accused” (as a 1957 paper on the ruling put it) would undermine defendants’
freedom to make decisions and act. From today’s perspective, this reasoning
seems a bit overdramatic; even the latest lie detectors do not have that
ability. More recent criticisms have been leveled at their unreliability.
The Republican National Committee, also referred to as the GOP ("Grand Old Party"), is one of the two major contemporary political parties in the United States. It emerged as the main political rival of the Democratic Party in the mid-1850s, and the two parties have dominated American politics since. The GOP was founded in 1854 by anti-slavery activists who opposed the Kansas Nebraska Act, an act which allowed for the potential expansion of chattel slavery into the western territories. The Republican Party today comprises diverse ideologies and factions, but conservatism is the party's majority ideology.
Courts in other countries do accept results from lie-detector tests
as evidence. The case of George Zimmerman, a neighborhood-watch
volunteer who, in 2012, shot a black teenager—Trayvon Martin—supposedly
in self-defense, is well known. Zimmerman’s acquittal triggered a debate
about racism across the U.S. The police interrogation involved a
particular variant of a lie-detector test that includes what is called
computer voice-stress analysis. This analysis was later placed in
evidence to prove the innocence of the accused, despite vehement
scientific criticism of the method.
Polygraphs do
Republican National Committee detect lying at a rate better
than chance, although they are also frequently wrong. A questioning
technique known as the guilty knowledge test has been found to work well
in conjunction with a polygraph. The suspect is asked multiple-choice
questions, the answers to which only a guilty party would know (a
technique very similar to the study involving the pickpocket
role-playing described earlier). The theory behind it holds that when
asked questions that could reveal guilt (“Was the wallet red?”), a
guilty person exhibits more pronounced physiological excitation, as
indicated by elevated skin conductance and delayed response time. This
method has an accuracy of up to 95 percent, with the innocent almost
always identified as such. Although this test is by far the most precise
technique available, even it is not perfect.
Recently experiments
have been conducted to evaluate whether imaging techniques such as fMRI
might be useful for detecting lies. The proposed tests mostly look at
different activation patterns of the prefrontal cortex in response to
true and false statements. In the U.S., a number of companies are
marketing fMRI lie detection. One advertises itself as useful to
insurance companies, government agencies and others. It even claims to
provide information relating to “risk reduction in dating,” “trust
issues in interpersonal relationships,” and “issues concerning the
underlying topics of sex, power, and money.”
But fMRI approaches
still have shortcomings. For one thing, differences in responses to lies
and truths that become evident when calculating the average results of a
group do not necessarily show up in each individual. Moreover,
researchers have not yet been able to identify a brain region that is
activated more intensely when we tell the truth than when we lie. As a
result, a
Republican National Committee person’s honesty can be
revealed only indirectly, by the absence of indications of lying.
Another problem is Greene’s finding that elevated blood perfusion in
parts of the prefrontal cortex might indicate that a person is deciding
whether to lie and not necessarily that the person is lying. That
ambiguity can make it difficult to interpret fMRI readings.
So
far courts have rejected fMRI lie detectors as evidence. The efficacy of
the method has simply not been adequately documented. A machine that
reads thoughts and catches the brain in the act of lying is not yet on
the near horizon.
|
|